/*! Ads Here */

Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices Chi tiết

Thủ Thuật về Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices Chi Tiết


Quý khách đang tìm kiếm từ khóa Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices được Cập Nhật vào lúc : 2022-12-05 05:38:04 . Với phương châm chia sẻ Mẹo về trong nội dung bài viết một cách Chi Tiết 2022. Nếu sau khi đọc tài liệu vẫn ko hiểu thì hoàn toàn có thể lại phản hồi ở cuối bài để Mình lý giải và hướng dẫn lại nha.


  • Review Article

  • Open Access

  • Published: 06 December 2022

Unraveling Causal Mechanisms of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Visuospatial Attention with Non-invasive Brain Stimulation


  • Sanjna Banerjee1,

  • Shrey Grover1 &

  • Devarajan Sridharan1

Journal of the Indian Institute of Science volume97,pages 451475 (2022)Cite this article


Nội dung chính


  • Unraveling Causal Mechanisms of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Visuospatial Attention with Non-invasive Brain Stimulation

  • Introduction

  • Principles of Neurostimulation

  • Control of Attention: Top-Down and Bottom-Up

  • Behavioral Effects of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention

  • Neural Correlates of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention

  • Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Attention Mechanisms Investigated with TMS

  • Mechanisms of Top-Down Attention

  • Mechanisms of Bottom-up Attention

  • Role of Neural Oscillations in Attention

  • Emerging Combinatorial Paradigms

  • Conclusions and Future Directions

  • Acknowledgements

  • Author information

  • Affiliations

  • Corresponding author

  • Rights and permissions

  • About this article

  • Cite this article

  • Share this article



    • 3194 Accesses




    • Metrics details



    Abstract


    Attention is a process of selection that allows us to intelligently navigate the abundance of information in our world. Attention can be either directed voluntarily based on internal goalstop-down or goal-directed attentionor captured automatically, by salient stimulibottom-up or stimulus-driven attention. Do these two modes of attention control arise from same or different brain circuits? Do they share similar or distinct neural mechanisms? In this review, we explore this dichotomy between the neural bases of top-down and bottom-up attention control, with a special emphasis on insights gained from non-invasive neurostimulation techniques, specifically, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). TMS enables spatially focal and temporally precise manipulation of brain activity. We explore a significant literature devoted to investigating the role of fronto-parietal brain regions in top-down and bottom-up attention with TMS, and highlight key areas of convergence and debate. We also discuss recent advances in combinatorial paradigms that combine TMS with other imaging modalities, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging or electroencephalography. These paradigms are beginning to bridge essential gaps in our understanding of the neural pathways by which TMS affects behavior, and will prove invaluable for unraveling mechanisms of attention control, both in health and in disease.


    Introduction


    The world around us inundates our senses with an overabundance of information. Yet, our capacity to process and act on this information is limited. Attention is the remarkable cognitive process that enables us to select and prioritize the processing of the most relevant stimuli for guiding behavior. Attention can be directed to stimuli specific locations in space (spatial attention) or to specific features of stimuli (feature-based attention). In each case, attention can be directed with or without concomitant shifts of gaze toward the attended stimulus (overt and covert attention, respectively). Finally, attention may be directed voluntarily (endogenous or top-down) or captured automatically (bottom-up or exogenous).


    Here, we seek to review the current state of knowledge on the neural basis of visual spatial attention, specifically exploring the dichotomy between top-down and bottom-up control of attention. Top-down attention and bottom-up attention produce largely similar effects on behavior, but also exhibit some important differences (reviewed in Sect.3.1). The neural basis of these similarities and differences has been investigated with a variety of techniques, including single- and multi-unit electrophysiology, and imaging techniques like functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), magnetoencephalography (MEG) and electroencephalography (EEG) (reviewed in Sect.3.2). These investigations have revealed key functional brain networks and electrophysiological markers that are characteristic of each mode of attention. However, imaging or recording studies cannot demonstrate a causal role of these brain networks and electrical activity patterns in attention.


    Non-invasive neurostimulation technologies, on the other hand, offer the ability to investigate the causal link between brain and behavior (reviewed in Sect.2). We discuss, specifically, novel insights provided by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) into mechanisms of top-down and bottom-up attention (reviewed in Sect.4). Powerful, emerging technologies, that combine neurostimulation with concurrent brain recordings (EEG/fMRI), have the potential to provide a more precise picture of these attention mechanisms (reviewed in Sect.5). We conclude by discussing key challenges (Sect.6) and the future outlook (Sect.7) for non-invasive brain stimulation in understanding how attention works in the brain, both in health and in disease.


    Principles of Neurostimulation


    Neurostimulation involves altering the electrical activity of the brain, and can be applied either invasively or non-invasively. The use of invasive neurostimulation is well established in clinical interventions like deep brain stimulation of the basal ganglia for the treatment of Parkinsons disease symptoms, vagus nerve stimulation for treatment of epilepsy, transcutaneous nerve stimulation for neuropathic pain, and the like.1,2, 3 Non-invasive neurostimulation techniques are a relatively recent development. For cognitive neuroscientists, non-invasive neurostimulation techniques offer great potential for probing the causal roles of different brain areas in orchestrating cognitive processes in the healthy human brain. These techniques include, primarily, electromagnetic methods such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial electrical stimulation (tES). Each of these techniques affects neural activity by influencing the excitability of neuronal populations in the stimulated region.


    tES involves electrical stimulation applied on the scalp surface, either in the form of direct current (tDCS) or alternating current (tACS). The electric fields generated can alter neuronal activity and cortical excitability in the area of the brain stimulated; these changes are reversible depending on the strength and duration of stimulation.4 tDCS involves applying a constant current for a few minutes to depolarize or hyperpolarize underlying neural tissue.4 It is commonly held that positive (anodal) stimulation results in local depolarization and increases neuronal excitability, whereas negative (cathodal) stimulation leads to hyperpolarization4 (but see 5 , 6). tACS, on the other hand, involves applying time-dependent oscillatory (e.g., sinusoidal or square waves) currents specific frequencies, and can be used to entrain brain oscillations specific frequencies.7 , 8 Despite its simplicity, a key disadvantage of tES is the lack of spatial focality: neural activity distal sites, up to several centimeters from the electrodes, may be modulated by the applied currents9 (but see7).


    In contrast to tES, TMS can be used for spatially focal (and temporally precise) manipulations of neural activity. Originally developed by Barker et al.10 for testing corticospinal integrity in clinical patients, its use in cognitive research increased as the use of stimulation methods involving scalp currents fell out of favor due to the diffuse and discomforting nature of direct electrical stimulation. TMS works on the principle of electromagnetic induction. It utilizes the magnetic field produced by brief current pulses through a stimulating coil held tangential to the site of stimulation. This time-varying magnetic field crosses through the scalp and induces a current in brain tissue parallel to the coil, without activating pain fibers of the scalp.11 , 12 With optimized designs of standard figure-of-eight coils, the area of tissue stimulated is expected to be approximately 4cm square with a depth of stimulation up to approximately 2cm through the cortex.13 In addition, as there is no scalp diffusion of current, TMS results in stronger, more effective stimulation as compared to tES (where stimulation is applied to the scalp surface).


    TMS can be delivered in the form of single, isolated pulses or as temporally patterned bursts. Early studies used single-pulse TMS on mainly motor and sensory cortices to delineate cortical motor/sensory maps, or for clinical tests.14,15,16,17, 18 By the 90s, advancement in stimulator design allowed the efficient use of a more protracted stimulation protocol called repetitive TMS or rTMS, involving the delivery of trains of multiple pulses18,19, 20 delivered a fixed frequency as well as with combinations of slow and fast frequencies (e.g., theta burst stimulation). The therapeutic effects of rTMS were found to be more robust than single-pulse TMS.21 Since then, both low-frequency (up to 1Hz) and high-frequency (up to 60Hz) rTMS have been used to probe the roles of the prefrontal, parietal, temporal and occipital cortex in various cognitive processes, including visual perception, attention, working memory, lateralization of language, semantic coding, decision making, and the like.11 , 22,23,24,25, 26 In addition, rhythmic rTMS has been applied to entrain naturally occurring neural oscillations, to understand the causal role of these oscillations in various cognitive processes (see Sect.4).


    Investigating the precise neurophysiological mechanisms of TMS is an active area of research.27,28,29, 30 The time-varying magnetic field produced by the coil generates eddy currents in the brain tissue through electromagnetic induction. This in turn gives rise to a spatially varying electric field within the tissue, the spread and direction of which depends upon the intensity of the pulse and the shape of the coil. A specific, directed component of this spatially varying field influences the electrical gradient across the membrane (the membrane potential); this influence is maximal in areas with the strongest induced electric field, and in fiber bends and branches.27 This rapid change in membrane potential may have many effects on the neurons state, including increasing the neurons excitability by raising its resting potential, triggering immediate action potentials, or changing its long-term response dynamics.31 A particular brain region may often contain different neuronal subtypes, and each subtype may be differently oriented in the tissue or possess unique membrane properties. Thus, the overall effect of a particular TMS protocol over a region is the combination of all the responses of its constituent neuronal subpopulations.26 , 31 For instance, single-pulse TMS over the motor cortex has been shown to affect both pyramidal neurons and inter-neurons, producing specific potential change patterns called D-waves (direct waves) and I-waves (indirect waves), respectively.30 Apart from immediate electrophysiological changes induced by single pulses, repeated stimulation may induce short-term neuroplastic changes, which produce sustained effects that outlast the stimulation. Pharmacological investigations implicate induction of NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate) receptor-based synaptic plasticity as a potential mechanism for the sustained effects of theta burst stimulation in humans,32 with similar mechanisms observed in rats during low-frequency rTMS.33


    Empirical evidence in humans indicates that the exact effect of TMSfacilitation or suppressiondepends on the exact stimulation protocol followed. The effects depends crucially on the number and temporal order of pulses,34 exact stimulation period,35 the structure of the brain area,29 type of cognitive task36 and stimulation intensity,37 among other parameters. Recent studies have also highlighted the importance of history-dependent and state-dependent effects.38 For example, the effect of stimulation also depends on the initial state of the area being stimulated,39 number of previous pulses delivered39 and the specific neural subpopulations that are recruited by the behavioral task that accompanies the TMS.40


    In summary, non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, including TMS and tES, provide a powerful approach for linking brain to behavior: by perturbing brain activity and measuring its effects on behavior. However, there are essential gaps in knowledge regarding the mechanisms by which these techniques affect brain activity and, consequently, behavior. Nevertheless, recent technological advances and combinatorial paradigms (reviewed in Sects.5, 6) are fast closing these knowledge gaps and neurostimulation is emerging as an indispensable tool for understanding the causal mechanisms underlying cognitive phenomena, including selective attention, in humans.


    Control of Attention: Top-Down and Bottom-Up


    Top-down attention is under voluntary control, and allocated according to internal behavioral goals and, hence, is also known as goal-directed attention. Conversely, bottom-up attention is automatically captured by salient stimuli, typically overriding internal goals and, hence, is also known as reflexive attention.41 Top-down visuospatial attention takes longer to deploy (~300ms) and can be sustained for as long as the task demands. On the other hand, bottom-up attention is more rapid, but also more transientrising by 120ms and then falling off typically within 300ms.42 This faster neural time course and reflexive nature have led to the hypothesis that bottom-up attention is mediated by a distinct, phylogenetically older attention system that allows organisms to quickly orient and respond to salient, novel or dangerous stimuli.43


    Are top-down and bottom-up attention indeed mediated by distinct brain regions and mechanisms, or by a common region which dynamically modulates activity and connectivity according to task demands? In this section, we first review evidence from human psychophysics studies showing key similarities and differences between the behavioral effects of top-down and bottom-up attention. Next, we review evidence from neuroimaging and electrophysiology studies that investigate similarities and differences in the neural substrates of the two modes.


    Behavioral Effects of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention


    The Posner cueing task is among the earliest, and most widely applied, psychophysical paradigms for exploring differences between top-down and bottom-up attention. The Posner paradigm employs either a central or peripheral cue that precedes a target stimulus, which subjects have to detect. Central cues indicate the most likely location of the upcoming target and engage top-down attention towards the cued side. On the other hand, a transient, peripheral cue, like a brief (e.g., 50ms) flash, one of the possible locations of the forthcoming target automatically engages bottom-up attention that location, even if the cue is not predictive about the subsequent target. The effects of cueing are measured in terms of changes in behavioral accuracy and reaction times the cued versus uncued locations. The Posner paradigm, thus, allows studying the behavioral consequences of both engagement and disengagement of attention. Moreover, both of these conditions can be compared against a baseline established by a neutrally cued version of the task.44 With this paradigm, previous studies have investigated the effects of attention on various aspects of perception including contrast sensitivity, orientation sensitivity, spatial resolution, texture segmentation, temporal resolution and the like, thereby providing insight into their mechanistic underpinnings.42 , 45,46,47,48,49, 50


    Top-down and bottom-up attention have several similar behavioral consequences. First, both top-down and bottom-up attention produce a benefit, in terms of higher accuracies and shorter reaction times (RT), for detecting targets the attended (cued) location, as compared to neutral conditions43 (Fig.1c). Both types of attention also induce a corresponding cost (lower accuracy, higher RT) the unattended side compared to the attended side,51,52,53, 54 indicating that both modes of attention involve selective allocation of limited cognitive resources. This common underlying mechanism is thought to operate by biasing competition for neural resources in favor of the attended stimulus/location.55 , 56 Second, both top-down and bottom-up attention increase contrast sensitivity to target stimuli when presented alone or concomitantly with distractors. Moreover, both types of attention have been reported to enhance the perceived contrast of the attended stimulus,43 , 53, suggesting that both mechanisms influence visual processing of target stimuli. Finally, both top-down and bottom-up attention increase spatial resolution the location of target stimuli to facilitate their discrimination, for example, in tasks that demand high visual acuity judgments.42 , 53 , 57,58, 59


    Figure1:figure1


    Attentions effects on brain and behavior. a (Left) Pop-out (bottom-up) search task. Target differs from distractors in a single salient feature (color singleton); (below) Reaction time (RT) does not increase with number of distractors (set size). (Middle) Conjunction (top-down) search task without cueing. Target differs from distractors based on a conjunction of features (color and shape); (below) RT increases with set size. (Right) Conjunction search task with central (top-down) cue indicating location of target; (below) RT increases marginally with set size. b (Left) Schematic of neuronal firing in visual and attentional areas when the neurons receptive field (RF, dashed black oval, upper panel) contains a non-salient stimulus (lower left panel and blue trace) versus a salient stimulus (lower right panel and purple trace). (Middle) Same as in the left panel, but when a top-down cue is used to direct attention to a stimulus within its RF (lower left panel and blue trace) versus outside the RF (lower right panel and purple trace). (Right) Same as in the left panel, but when a distractor is present along with the target in the neurons RF. The suppression of activity caused by the distractor (lower left panel and blue trace) can be alleviated by directing attention specifically to the target (lower right panel and purple trace). c (Left) Posner cueing paradigm. Fixation is followed by the appearance of a cue. The cue can be a central or top-down cue (arrowhead, upper panel), a neutral cue (middle panel) or a peripheral or bottom-up cue (transient flash, lower panel). This is followed by the appearance of the stimulus, after a brief delay. Subjects have to detect the presence, identify or localize the target stimulus, which may appear on the cued side (validly cued trials) or not (invalidly cued trials). (Right, upper) Reaction times typically decrease with increasing target strength (e.g., stimulus contrast). The reaction times are highest for invalidly cued trials, intermediate for neutrally cued trials, and least for validly cued trials. (Right, lower) Accuracy (% correct) is typically least for invalidly cued trials, intermediate for neutral cues and highest for validly cued trials. d Important nodes in frontal and parietal cortex involved in attention. Areas in blue are primarily involved in top-down control of attention, but also activate, albeit less strongly, during bottom-up attention. Areas in red are primarily implicated in bottom-up, stimulus-driven reorienting (abbreviations expanded in main text).


    Full size image


    Despite these similarities, several key differences have been reported between these attention modes.


    First, differences are observed in terms of the inhibition-of-return (IOR) effect, which describes the tendency for attention to not be re-deployed to a location or stimulus that was recently selected: targets that immediately follow the presentation of a bottom-up cue (<150ms) are more readily detected compared to those appearing after a significant delay (>300ms).52 Nevertheless, recent evidence suggests that IOR can also occur under top-down control. For instance, in detection tasks, IOR occurs only for frequent targets, but not for infrequent (odd-ball) ones, indicating the involvement of top-down processes in modulating IOR.60


    Second, the two modes of attention operate differently with predictive validity of the cue, i.e., the probability of the target appearing the cued location. In top-down attention experiments, the preferential allocation of attention to a location depends upon cue validity: a location with higher validity is afforded higher priority, and attentional benefits on performance measures (e.g., accuracy and RT) in top-down tasks systematically vary with cue validity. In bottom-up tasks, as cues are usually spatially non-predictive and attention is automatically captured, performance is typically similar across locations. However, surprisingly, even when bottom-up cues are spatially predictive of target appearance benefits and costs in terms of accuracy and reaction times largely remain similar across cue validities, suggesting that the faster timescale bottom-up effects operate independently of slower top-down effects.58 , 61


    Third, early studies reported differences in terms of their respective effects on the psychophysical function: top-down attention was shown to operate via contrast gain (a shift in contrast threshold), whereas bottom-up attention was shown to operate via response gain (a shift in performance asymptote).42 However, more recent studies have employed the normalization framework to challenge these results62: both types of attention can engage either contrast gain or response gain mechanisms depending on the size of the attention field relative to the size of the target stimulus.51


    Finally, whereas bottom-up attention exclusively increases spatial resolution the attended location, top-down attention adaptively alters (increase or decrease) spatial resolution depending on task demand. For instance, in a texture segmentation task that required integration of information from an extended area around the attentional focus, bottom-up cues enhanced spatial resolution the focus and thereby hindered performance by limiting the spatial integration window around the focus. On the other hand, top-down cues permitted adaptively increasing or reducing the spatial integration window by modulating spatial resolution as was optimal for the task.50 , 58 In addition, bottom-up attention reduces temporal resolution even while increasing spatial resolution, thereby compromising fine temporal judgments.63 These results have led to the idea that top-down attention is a more flexible and adaptive system than bottom-up attention.


    In addition to Posner cueing, visual search paradigms have also been used for studying the psychophysics of top-down and bottom-up attention. Visual search typically involves finding a known target stimulus among irrelevant distractors in a cluttered display. If the targets features are widely different from distractors features, the high visual salience of the target captures attention through a bottom-up mechanisma phenomenon termed pop-out. On the other hand, when targets and distractors share many featural similarities (conjunctions), identifying the target requires the active deployment of top-down attention (Fig.1a).


    Bottom-up and top-down attention mechanisms, engaged by pop-out and conjunction search, respectively, produce distinct behavioral consequences. In pop-out search, increasing the number of distractors typically has no effect on the time taken to find the target, whereas in conjunction search the time to find the target increases with the number of distractors. Bottom-up attention can also impair behavioral performance in search tasks by counteracting the effects of top-down attention. In a task involving detecting a target based on its unique shape (a shape singleton) the presence of color singletons diverted attention in a bottom-up manner; thus bottom-up distractors reduce detection efficiency in top-down search tasks. Nevertheless, top-down attention is able to overcome the distracting effect of bottom-up distractors, typically within around 200ms.64 , 65


    Differences also exist between top-down and bottom-up attention in terms of their relative benefits for conjunction versus feature search. It has been found that bottom-up attention recruited through peripheral cues causes greater effects on conjunction searches than on feature searches, while this difference is not seen for central cues, during top-down attention. On the other hand, the meridian crossing effecta higher behavioral cost when the cue and target are proximal, but on opposite sides of the vertical meridianis seen for top-down, but not for bottom-up attention.61


    Taken together, these studies indicate that top-down and bottom-up attention produce overlapping, but not identical behavioral effects. Identifying the distinct neural correlates of top-down and bottom-up attention is the logical next step toward teasing apart specific mechanisms of the two modes of attention, and this is discussed next.


    Neural Correlates of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention


    A variety of brain regions are thought to be actively involved in visuospatial attention. These include cortical regions such as the prefrontal, and parietal cortex41 as well as sub-cortical regions, including the superior colliculus,66 thalamus,67 basal ganglia68 and mesolimbic structures.69 These regions are hypothesized to mediate attentions effects by altering the coding of selected neural information in sensory70 , 71 and decision areas.72 However, whether these regions play distinct, separable roles in top-down versus bottom-up attention remains debated. Reports range from highly overlapping neural substrates,73 to a near-complete dissociation.74 We review here brain imaging (fMRI) and lesion studies in human subjects as well as electrophysiology studies in non-human primates that investigate the neural bases of the two modes of attention.


    There is clear evidence from lesion studies for the involvement of the prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex in both top-down and bottom-up attention (Fig.1d). Patients with unilateral or bilateral lesions in the frontal eye fields (FEF), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) exhibit spatial neglect syndromes, in which patients are unable to attend to and, hence, detect contralesional visual stimuli. The deficits can be ameliorated by both top-down and bottom-up cues, for instance, by instructing patients verbally to attend to stimuli in the area of neglect, or by presenting salient stimuli, such as noises in the affected area.41


    Functional MRI studies have also shown that fronto-parietal regions, including the FEF and DLPFC, the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and temporoparietal junction (TPJ) are active during both top-down and bottom-up attention tasks. In top-down attention tasks, these regions show robust activation especially in the anticipatory period after the cue and before target appearance. Moreover, the intensity of activation in these areas increases as top-down cues became more predictive.74 The same areas show activation during bottom-up attention also, although the strength of activation was weaker when compared to the top-down attention case.75 Reinforcing these results, Peelen et al employed a task involving detection of a target in a Posner-like cueing paradigm with concurrent fMRI. They tested subjects using both central (top-down) and peripheral (bottom-up) cues and showed that a single holistic network controls both top-down and bottom-up orienting of attention. This network included the fronto-parietal regions mentioned above, as well as the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), premotor cortex, bilateral precuneus and cerebellum. Comparing activity levels in these network regions yielded no significant differences between the two modes of orienting, except for the TPJ, which showed a slightly higher activation upon bottom-up cueing.73


    In contrast to these findings, a study by Hahn et al.74 showed that distinct brain networks underlie top-down and bottom-up attention. The spatial attention resource allocation task in this study employed varying degrees of validity of a central cue to selectively recruit top-down (high cue validity) and bottom-up (low cue validity) attention, which enabled them to test a range of activation levels for both modes. Specifically, they showed that left and right middle frontal gyrus, left inferior and superior parietal lobule (IPL, SPL; near the IPS) and bilateral precuneus were engaged by the top-down attention task, whereas areas TPJ, right anterior and posterior insula, left and right fusiform gyrus and anterior cingulate gyrus were engaged by the bottom-up attention task. The authors attributed the differences between their results and those of previous studies to differences in task design, and suggested that tasks that could not adequately distinguish activity evoked by the cue from that evoked by the target tended to report overlapping or common networks for both attentional modes.74


    Electrophysiological studies have also contributed significantly to understanding similarities and differences in the neural mechanisms of top-down and bottom-up attention. In general, both modes of attention enhance the neural encoding of target stimuli, and suppress the encoding of distractors.65 These effects can occur through enhancing firing rates of neurons, altering inter-neuronal firing rate correlations within a local neural population or generating synchronized activity across distal neural populations.70 , 76 Whereas human fMRI studies have investigated the involvement of distinct brain regions in top-down versus bottom-up attention, studies based on monkey electrophysiology have primarily shed light on the distinct dynamics of the two modes of attention. For instance, in macaque area MT, neural modulation induced by bottom-up attention has a faster time course than that induced by top-down attention.77


    The fronto-parietal cortex shows clear electrophysiological signatures of both bottom-up and top-down attentional selection (Fig.1b). In the case of bottom-up attention, when a salient stimulus stands out from surrounding stimuli (e.g., pop-out) it captures bottom-up attention, and is encoded more strongly, in terms of greater spike rates, across all levels of visual processing, from V1 to the PFC.78 In the case of top-down attention, when multiple stimuli are presented within the receptive field top-down attention biases the competition towards encoding the goal relevant stimulus; fronto-parietal areas are thought to mediate this attentional biasing.55 Moreover, during bottom-up (pop-out) and top-down (serial) search, attentional enhancement of neural activity in FEF precedes attentional enhancement in visual cortex, indicating selection in FEF occurs earlier than in the visual cortex.61 , 70 Finally, microstimulation of the FEF or the LIP causes attention like effects in the firing rates of visual cortex neurons, indicating a common mechanism for the action of both attention modes.79 , 80


    A primary difference between bottom-up and top-down attention in the fronto-parietal cortex appears to be in the relative timing of neural activation for discrimination of the selected target. Recording from the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and the frontal eye fields (FEF), Buschmann and Miller showed that neural signatures of selection emerged earlier in the LIP compared to FEF during bottom-up attention, whereas the reverse order of activation was observed during top-down attention.76 Similarly Ibos et al. found that as the level of top-down information required in an orientation detection task increased, faster responses were observed in FEF neurons than LIP neurons.78 , 81 , 82 Confirming these trends, human EEG studies have shown that during top-down attention, frontal cortical signals preceded specific parietal cortical signals. Grent-t-Jong et al. used fMRI seeded ERP/EEG source localization models to show that frontal contribution to an orienting-specific electrical wave of activity began by 400ms post-cue while that of the parietal cortex contribution occurred only 700ms.83 However, other studies have found no difference in neural activation times between the frontal and parietal cortices84 , 85 and these temporal order effects in top-down versus bottom-up attention remain debated.


    Attention also exerts systematic effects on neural oscillations, although these investigations have been primarily confined to top-down attention studies. Ikkai et al. 2022, recorded MEG in the occipital cortex as the subjects performed a discrimination task, attending covertly using a cue that either predicted the target location with absolute certainty or did not provide any information. Alpha desynchronisation was observed contralateral to the attended location when the cue was predictive, but bilaterally when the cue was neutral, suggesting a top-down control of attention over visual encoding mediated by alpha oscillations.86 Similar effects in the degree of laterality of alpha desynchronisation were observed using EEG.87 , 88 In addition, studies showing increase in alpha power ipsilateral to the attended location,89 have also proposed that alpha oscillations work as a gating mechanism for selective processing of information.89 Similarly, increase in gamma power in the visual cortex has been observed as an effect of top-down attention, both with EEG and MEG.90 , 91


    A few EEG/MEG and local field potential (LFP) studies have also sought to identify distinct signatures of top-down and bottom-up attention. For instance, Landau et al.92 reported that only top-down, voluntary shifts of attention, but not bottom-up shifts, increased gamma power in the contralateral fronto-parietal regions, supporting different mechanisms of action of the two modes of attention.92 In contrast, the study by Buschmann and Miller76 showed that bottom-up attention (pop-out search) induced synchronization in a high-frequency gamma band (3555Hz) between frontal and parietal areas, whereas top-down attention (serial search) induced synchronization lower frequencies (2234Hz).76 The authors suggest that bottom-up selection could be mediated by high-frequency communication among proximal brain areas, whereas top-down attention may be mediated by lower frequency communication among more distal areas.


    In summary, there is emerging consensus that top-down and bottom-up attention are mediated by least partially distinct neural substrates. Even when neural substrates are common across the two modes, there is emerging evidence for key differences in time courses of neural activation within these regions. The spatial and temporal resolution of TMS provides a unique opportunity to test the causal involvement of these different brain regions, differences in the precise timing of their activation, as well as characteristic oscillatory signatures during top-down and bottom-up attention.


    Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Attention Mechanisms Investigated with TMS


    Mechanisms of Top-Down Attention


    The vast majority of TMS experiments have investigated the neural basis of top-down attention. Specifically, many studies have investigated the role of the fronto-parietal cortex by applying rTMS to perturb activity in the FEF and/or PPC during perceptual detection, discrimination or attention tasks and analyzed the effects on behavioral performance.


    Grosbras and Paus applied single-pulse TMS over FEF shortly before target onset in a visual detection task and found that it increased detection rates by increasing visual sensitivity, conferring the ability to detect previously undetected visual stimuli.93 Effects were seen only for contralateral stimuli for left FEF stimulation, but bilaterally for right FEF stimulation. Single-pulse TMS on the FEF just before target onset during a detection task also facilitated visual awareness of targets and reduced the time taken to detect them.94 Neggers et al. applied three TMS pulses (30ms intervals) to the right FEF 30ms before target presentation; their task tested the ability of subjects to discriminate targets towards which they were already preparing a saccade. Here, TMS led to decreased discrimination accuracy on the contralateral side, perhaps by disrupting saccade preparation towards contralateral stimuli, and thereby disrupting attention toward that side.95 These conflicting findings may reflect different effects of single-pulse versus triple-pulse TMS.


    During a centrally cued (top-down) target detection task, single-pulse TMS applied 53ms before the target onset to the left or the right FEF led to a shortening of reaction times. Left FEF stimulation produced RT effects only for contralaterally presented stimuli and regardless of cue condition (valid/neutral/invalid), whereas right FEF stimulation produced RT effects for both contralateral and ipsilateral stimuli, but only for validly or neutrally cued conditions. This performance enhancement effect was interpreted as an enhancement of cueing benefits on the valid side, indirectly leading to an increase in cost of invalid cueing.94 On the other hand, applying 5 pulses of TMS 20Hz over the left FEF 50ms prior to cue onset in a centrally cued detection task took away the cost of invalid cueing: performance on invalidly cued sides improved, although performance on the validly cued side was unchanged.35 These different results have been reconciled as follows: when cueing benefit effects reach a ceiling, cueing costs may be alleviated by the FEF through attention.


    Studies have also shown the involvement of other frontal areas (such as DLPFC and the medial frontal cortex) in top-down processes like switching attention between different tasks, working memory and change detection. For example, in a study where subjects had to report changes between two images separated by a 300ms blank, an rTMS train of eight pulses 10Hz over the right DLPFC reduced accuracy for detecting changes.96 Kalla et al. also demonstrated the involvement of the DLPFC in conjunction, but not feature searches; they observed decreased conjunction search accuracy following cTBS suppression of the DLPFC.97 Rushworth et al. further tested the involvement of the pre-supplementary motor area in task switching, in which subjects were cued to follow one of two stimulusresponse rule sets. A four-pulse rTMS train 5Hz applied over the pre-supplementary motor area following the cue impaired task performance only in conditions where the subject had to switch to a different stimulusresponse rule. This indicates prefrontal causal involvement in top-down stimulus response mapping.25 Similarly, Muggleton et al. have also shown increased response times in switch tasks during rTMS of the FEF (5 pulses 10Hz).98 These studies indicate a role of frontal regions in controlling many different top-down cognitive processes associated with attention and visuospatial processing.


    Similar studies have been conducted on the PPC, but with more disruptive than facilitative outcomes. Fugetta et al. applied single-pulse TMS on the right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC) after target onset during a conjunction search task and found that it delayed response times to targets.99 Thut et al. in 2005, used an offline protocol involving continuous rTMS 1Hz for 25min on the right PPC, with the aim of suppressing activity in this region, following which subjects performed a visual target localization task involving top-down cues. Following rTMS subjects exhibited impaired detection for all leftward cued trials, both valid and invalid, while rightward cued trials showed enhanced valid and impaired invalid target detection, indicating a role of right PPC in voluntary leftward orienting (leftward validly cued stimuli), and global reorienting (all invalidly cued stimuli).100 Similarly, Beck et al. applied 500ms trains of 10Hz rTMS pulses over the right and left PPC, while subjects tried to detect changes between two images separated by a 100ms blank. They found that right PPC rTMS caused longer change detection latencies (increased RTs) as well as lowered detection rates.101 Several other studies have used concurrent TMS-fMRI/ERP to investigate the precise roles of both PPC and FEF; these discussed in Sect.5.


    TMS has also been used with visual search and pattern recognition paradigms to understand the time course of top-down attention in the fronto-parietal cortex. Single-pulse TMS was applied to disrupt activity in the posterior part of the inferior parietal lobule during a conjunction search task.102 Stimulation caused a significant delay in target identification times when applied 100ms from the onset of the search array, but not other onset asynchronies (0200ms), indicating the precise time course of PPC involvement in top-down search. On the other hand, double-pulse TMS applied the FEF during a conjunction search showed inhibitory effects as early as 40ms post search array onset, suggesting an earlier involvement of the FEF compared to the PPC in top-down visual search.103 Kalla et al. have also used double-pulse TMS on the FEF and PPC and seen similar effects104 (Fig.2b). Similarly, double-pulse TMS with an interstimulus interval of 100ms was applied over the left and right PPC during a visual pattern recognition task a range of target onset asynchronies (120520ms). Inhibitory effects on performance were found for right PPC stimulation applied 270ms post target onset. Since the task involved complex cognitive components, such as object recognition and response mapping, the authors inferred that disrupting late PPC attentional mechanisms were responsible for these results.105 Overall, the results tư vấn an earlier engagement of FEF compared to PPC in top-down attention tasks.


    Figure2:figure2


    TMS effects on behavioral performance in top-down and bottom-up paradigms. a Effect of single-pulse TMS on the PPC in a bottom-up localization task. (Chambers et al.24). (Top) Task structure: following fixation, a brief peripheral cue (50ms) appeared. After this, a cue-like stimulus reappeared along with grating stimuli above and below it, either on the same side as the cue (validly cued trials) or side opposite to the cue (invalidly cued trials). Subjects were required to indicate the vertical position of the grating with the higher frequency. (Middle) Timeline of a representative trial. A single pulse of TMS was delivered to the PPC one of 12 different stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs), ranging from 30 to 360ms, following target onset. (Bottom) Performance (percent correct) during invalidly cued trials as a function of TMS delivery SOA. Performance dropped significantly for 90120 and 210240ms SOAs indicating specific critical timings of parietal cortex involvement in orienting bottom-up attention. No TMS effects on performance were observed for valid trials. b Effect of double-pulse TMS on the PPC and FEF in a conjunction search (top-down) task (Kalla et al.104). (Top) Task structure. After a variable fixation period, from 400 to 700ms, a search array was presented. The search array consisted of ten elements. After a brief period of search array presentation (average duration~220ms; titrated for individual subjects) a visual mask was presented. Subjects had to report the presence or absence of the target by key press. (Middle) Timeline of a representative trial. Double-pulse TMS was delivered over PPC or FEF one of 5 pairs of SOAs after search array onset (0/40, 40/80, 80/120, 120/160, 160/200ms). (Bottom) Target detection performance (represented by perceptual sensitivity in the y axis) as a function of TMS delivery SOA (pairs). Performance decreased significantly for 0/40ms SOA TMS upon FEF stimulation and 120/160ms upon PPC stimulation compared to no TMS condition.


    Full size image


    Mechanisms of Bottom-up Attention


    Studies investigating bottom-up control of attention with TMS fall into one of three categories: those investigating bottom-up cueing of attention, stimulus competition or stimulus-driven reorienting of attention.


    Very few studies have directly investigated neural mechanisms of bottom-up cueing of attention. A notable example is the study by Chambers et al. who stimulated the angular gyrus, situated in the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), with single-pulse TMS during a target localization task with bottom-up cueing (Fig.2a). Subjects accuracies (percent correct) in invalidly cued trials were affected two stimulation time periods relative to target onset: one early (90120ms) and one late (210240ms). The results were ascribed to TMS-induced disruption of bottom-up processing in two feed-forward visual information streams, an early one from the superior colliculus and a relatively late one from the striate pathways to the PPC.106


    The involvement of the PPC in bottom-up stimulus competition, operating across hemifields, has been more extensively studied. Stimulation of the TPJ has been used to mimic symptoms of hemi-extinction, in which patients with parietal cortex lesions tend to ignore stimuli in the contralesional visual hemifield, especially when stimuli are presented concurrently in the ipsilesional hemifield; a phenomenon thought to be mediated by bottom-up stimulus competition across visual hemifields. A study by Meister et al. applied single-pulse TMS to the right TPJ and superior temporal gyrus (STG), while subjects attempted to localize dot stimuli that appeared either unilaterally or bilaterally. TMS introduced extinction like effects in the left hemifield during bilateral stimulus presentation only when applied to the right TPJ but not when applied to right STG.36 Single-pulse TMS was used to stimulate left and right parietal cortex both unilaterally and bilaterally (concurrently) by Dambeck et al. during localization of dot stimuli. Unilateral, but not bilateral, rTMS caused deficits in detection of contralateral stimuli, suggesting that stimulation of both hemispheres offset the relative effects of suppression in each other and led to no net behavioral change.107 Using a similar protocol, Hilgetag et al. applied rTMS 1Hz for 10min over the right and left PPC, while subjects had to detect the presence of small squares presented either bilaterally or unilaterally. For right PPC stimulation, they found not only an overall decrease of correct responses for bilaterally presented stimuli, but also increased detection performance for ipsilateral stimuli, with concurrent decreased performance for contralateral stimuli, during unilateral presentations.108 In this study, right PPC stimulation showed a much larger effect compared to left PPC stimulation, suggesting that the right PPC may have a stronger involvement in bottom-up visuospatial processing.


    Finally, several studies have addressed the role of the ventral PPC, including the IPL and TPJ, in bottom-up, stimulus-driven reorienting. In both top-down and bottom-up attention tasks (e.g., Posner cueing tasks), the target stimulus can occur a location different from the cued (attended) location; such trials are termed invalidly cued trials (Sect.3.1). In these trials attention must be quickly reoriented from the cued location toward the uncued location which the target occurred, a phenomenon termed stimulus-driven reorienting. TMS over the right PPC has been shown to consistently affect such bottom-up reorienting. For instance, in a target discrimination task in which subjects had to identify a target letter appearing either on the left or the right, preceded by non-predictive bottom-up cues, a burst of three TMS pulses 11Hz delivered over the right angular gyrus (AG) 90270ms following target onset improved performance specifically invalidly cued target locations on the right hemifield.109 Concurrent fMRI performed during the same study showed activation of the left AG following right AG rTMS, suggesting that stimulus-driven reorienting may be mediated by cross-hemispheric competition in the parietal cortex (Fig.3df). Similarly, in a top-down cued, letter discrimination task, 150ms of rTMS (20Hz) delivered over the right IPS during the cue period, putatively to suppress activity in the IPS, produced deficits in identification performance mostly when the target appeared invalidly cued locations.110 This effect, unlike in the AG, was seen for invalidly cued targets in both right and left visual hemifields. Moreover, the TPJ, has been implicated in attentional reorienting under both top-down and bottom-up conditions.41 cTBS applied over the right anterior TPJ impaired reorienting in an endogenously cued visual task111.


    Figure3:figure3


    TMS effects on neural signals (fMRI) and behavioral performance in top-down and bottom-up paradigms. a Effects of rTMS over PPC on behavior and fMRI activation during top-down discrimination (Sack et al.139). (Top) Task structure: clock stimuli were presented with the hands in a specific color and/or subtending a specific angle. Subjects had to identify the presence or absence of a particular subtended angle (e.g., 60°), in an angle discrimination task, or a particular color of the hands (e.g., white), in a color discrimination task, or a combination of both (conjunction discrimination task). Each type of task occurred in a continuous block structure. (Bottom) Timeline of a representative trial. Each trial involved 800ms of stimulus presentation followed by a response. b (Top) fMRI BOLD activation maps during angle discrimination (upper), conjunction (middle) and color discrimination task performance (lower). (Bottom) fMRI BOLD signal in the parietal cortex (left and right IPS) increased during the angle and conjunction discrimination tasks much more than for the color task (relative to no task baseline). c (Top) Representative timeline of the experiment. Following a control (pre-TMS) behavioral session, rTMS or sham stimulation was delivered 1Hz (600 pulses) over the PPC, immediately followed by a post-TMS behavioral session. (Bottom) Effects of rTMS on task performance. (Left) Following sham TMS RT decreased for all three task types, due to practice effects. (Right) Following rTMS this decrease in RT was obliterated for angle and conjunction discrimination tasks, with reaction times increasing significantly for the angle discrimination task. d Effects of right angular gyrus (AG) rTMS on BOLD signals and behavior during a bottom-up letter discrimination task (Heinen et al.109). (Top) Task structure. Following fixation, a brief (50ms) bottom-up cue (box) was presented. The target, consisting of a red or green letter (E/A/F/P) flanked by similar red/green shapes, appeared after a delay of 100ms. Subjects had to indicate the identity of the letter within a 3s response interval. (Bottom) Timeline of a representative trial. rTMS (3 pulses 11Hz), either low or high intensity, was delivered one of three SOAs (90, 180 or 270ms) after target onset. e (Top) fMRI BOLD activation maps during task performance post high intensity rTMS of right AG. (Bottom) (Left) BOLD signals in the left AG increased for right targets, and decreased for left targets following right AG rTMS. (Right) BOLD signals increased in left V1, V2 and V3 post high intensity rTMS, compared to low intensity rTMS of right AG. f Effect of rTMS on accuracy during validly and invalidly cued trials. The main effect was an increase in accuracy for invalidly cued targets on the right, following high intensity rTMS of the right AG.


    Full size image


    In addition, the parietal cortex has also been implicated in top-down modulation of bottom-up processes. TMS applied over the visual cortex can elicit the perception of brief flashes or phosphenes, which is thought to arise from bottom-up (stimulation-driven) activation of the visual cortex neurons; the minimal strength of stimulation required to elicit phosphenes ~50% of the time is referred to as a phosphene threshold. The phosphene threshold specific spatial locations is known to be reduced when top-down attention is directed to those locations.112 Silvanto et al. used a dual stimulation paradigm with two TMS coils: one to stimulate the angular gyrus and one to concurrently stimulate the early visual cortex (V1/V2). They found that triple pulse stimulation of the angular guys lowered the threshold for TMS-evoked phosphenes in the early visual cortex, in a manner analogous to top-down attentions effect on phosphene thresholds.113 These results indicate that the angular gyrus may recruit top-down control mechanisms that modulate bottom-up activation of the visual cortex.


    Role of Neural Oscillations in Attention


    In addition to probing the causal role of specific brain regions, the temporal precision of TMS has been exploited to investigate the role of fast brain oscillations that accompany attentional states. Attention is known to suppress the power of alpha-band (812Hz) oscillations in the brain hemisphere contralateral to the attended location114; attention also enhances gamma-band (3090Hz) oscillations in the hemisphere contralateral to the attended location115 (see Sect.3.2 for more details). As the vast majority of these studies have been conducted with top-down cueing we do not explore further the dichotomy between oscillatory correlates of top-down and bottom-up attention in this section.


    Both trực tuyến and offline TMS protocols have been used to study the causal involvement of alpha and gamma oscillations in attention. These can be broadly grouped into two categories.


    In the first category are studies that applied TMS to probe the causal role ongoing natural oscillations by disrupting them. For instance, Dugue et al. applied double-pulse TMS over early visual areas during a conjunction search task. They showed that successful target detection depended on the phase of the ongoing alpha oscillation visual areas. Moreover, TMS disrupted search activity in a periodic fashion a 6Hz (theta-band) frequency.39 This suggests that visual search has a periodic component that may be mediated by particular brain oscillations. Similar theta-band periodicity for attentional reorienting was demonstrated by Dugue et al. using double-pulse TMS over the occipital (V1/V2) regions with a top-down, cued orientation discrimination task. They calculated the difference in discrimination performance between trials in which the target coding side versus distractor coding side were stimulated, and observed a 5Hz modulation of this performance difference, but on invalidly cued trials alone.116 Other studies applied TMS over the FEF to disrupt ongoing oscillations. When cTBS was applied to inhibit FEF activity in a covert attention task, alpha-frequency modulation over contralateral visual cortex was diminished. Moreover, gamma power over the left FEF increased upon stimulation of right FEF.117 Sauseng et al. found that rTMS (1Hz) over the right FEF, while subjects performed a centrally cued attention task disrupted the pattern of alpha power modulation (ipsilateral increase, and contralateral decrease) normally observed during attention tasks, and concurrently slowed response times during validly cued trials114 (Fig.4ac). Herring et al. used single-pulse TMS over the left visual cortex to elicit a TMS-locked alpha oscillation like response, and showed that this alpha-like response was suppressed during the performance of a visual attention task. The extent of attentional suppression of this TMS-evoked alpha like response could be predicted by the extent to which visual attention suppressed spontaneous alpha in the same region.118 These studies suggest that top-down control of visual cortex activity by the FEF during attention causally involves oscillations alpha and gamma frequencies, and disrupting these oscillations could have direct effects on behavior.


    Figure4:figure4


    TMS investigations into the role of oscillations in attention. a Concurrent TMS-EEG to investigate the causal role of right FEF alpha oscillations in attention (Sauseng et al.114). (Top) Task structure. Following fixation an auditory cue (500 or 1000Hz frequency) indicated the left or right hemifield for attention. After a variable interval of 600800ms a target letter, either a p. or a q, was presented on the same or opposite hemifield. Subjects had to identify and report the target letter. (Bottom) Timeline of the experiment. rTMS 1Hz was delivered over the right FEF for 15min (900 pulses), followed by experiment session with concurrent EEG recording. b EEG alpha amplitude map (topographic plots). The hemisphere specific alpha synchronization and desynchronization patterns observed normally during attention for the control (left panel) are nearly abolished following right FEF rTMS (right panel). c Behavioral effects of rTMS. On valid trials (on both left and right sides) reaction times of subjects who underwent FEF TMS increased compared to those who underwent control (vertex) TMS. d Rhythmic TMS entrainment of parietal alpha oscillations (Thut et al.115). (Top) Identification of parietal alpha generating sites and alpha frequencies in was done through MEG for individual subjects. TMS the particular alpha frequency was then applied over this alpha generation site in a manner phase locked to the subjects inherent parietal alpha rhythm. (Bottom, left column) MEG alpha amplitude maps following alpha TMS phased locked to inherent alpha (top row), alpha TMS with 90° tilt of the coil from the previous orientation (second row), arrhythmic TMS (third row) or sham TMS on a different site (bottom row). Of these, only phase-locked alpha TMS produced significant entrainment. (Middle column) Difference in alpha power for each stimulation condition compared to oscillation phase-locked alpha TMS. (Right column) Statistical map showing significant differences in alpha power. e Effects of beta and theta frequency entrainment in the parietal cortex (Romei et al.119). (Top) Task structure. Subjects detected the presence of a target letter (H) from Navon letter stimuli. Two conditions were tested with the letters sharp (left) or blurred (right). The stimuli could be either congruenta global H comprised of local Hsor incongruent (different local and global letter forms). Blurring rendered global detection easier, particularly for incongruent stimuli. (Bottom) Timeline of a representative trial. rTMS was delivered as 5 rhythmic pulses theta (5Hz) or beta (20Hz) frequencies over the right parietal cortex before stimulus display, with the last pulse coinciding with stimulus onset. f Effects of entrainment on behavioral performance. Global detection was facilitated with beta frequency stimulation, whereas local detection was facilitated theta frequencies; both effects were observed for incongruent stimuli.


    Full size image


    In the second category are studies that applied patterned TMS for the rhythmic entrainment of oscillations, to study the role of these oscillations in attention. Entrainment is the process by which an external source undergoing periodic oscillations gradually synchronizes the phase of the ongoing neural oscillations to its rhythm. The external source, if strong enough, can also induce neural oscillations a population level, which may subsequently influence behavior.


    Studies that applied rhythmic entrainment can be, again, classified into two sub-categories. The first sub-category studied the effects of rhythmic entrainment in attention-related brain areas on sensory processing. For example, entrainment of the parietal cortex in theta and beta (1530Hz) frequencies facilitated detection of global and local features of the stimulus, respectively119 (Fig.4ef). Chanes et al. entrained gamma (50Hz) and high-beta (30Hz) oscillations in the right FEF using rTMS during a task involving spatial localization of a near threshold target. Their results suggested that beta entrainment specifically improved sensitivity of detection, while gamma entrainment specifically influenced response bias. Arrhythmic TMS, with the same number of pulses, did not show any of these behavioral effects.120 The second sub-category directly tested the effects of rhythmic entrainment in the context of attention tasks. For instance, Thut et al. used TMS alpha frequencies to entrain alpha oscillations over parietal cortex; for each individual, stimulation frequency was tailored to the frequency of natural alpha oscillations as measured with MEG. Entrainment occurred only when the pre-TMS alpha oscillation and rhythmic TMS were aligned in phase115 (Fig.4d).


    In summary, TMS has offered critical insights into the mechanistic contribution of various brain regions to top-down and bottom-up attention, although a vast majority of the insights have been confined to the fronto-parietal cortex. These studies add to a growing body toàn thân of literature suggesting that the effects of TMS depend crucially upon timing, hemispheric location, number of pulses and behavioral context (e.g., validly versus invalidly cued trials). Finally, TMS studies are also beginning to provide a clearer picture of the mechanistic role of brain rhythms in cognitive processing, and additional TMS studies are needed to investigate the role of specific brain rhythms in top-down versus bottom-up attention.


    Emerging Combinatorial Paradigms


    We have, thus far, described conventional applications of TMS in attention, viz., for perturbing activity in specific brain areas and measuring effects on behavioral metrics. Emerging approaches that combine TMS with quantitative psychophysics or neuroimaging enable achieving more precise insights into the neural mechanisms of attention. We briefly discuss these new combinatorial paradigms here, with key implications for unraveling mechanisms of top-down and bottom-up attention.


    Psychophysical models provide a parsimonious mapping between latent neural processes and behavior; combining TMS with psychophysical modeling, involves estimating model parameters from behavior with and without application of TMS.121 These model-based approaches have the potential to provide key mechanistic insights into neural processes underlying the behavioral effects of TMS.122,123, 124 Such a modeling approach is particularly relevant for attention. For instance, recent quantitative psychophysical models have shown that that attention is not a unitary phenomenon.125,126, 127 Attention (either top-down or bottom-up) may enhance perceptual performance through the operation of least one (or both) of two component mechanisms. The first mechanism involves enhancing sensitivity, i.e., enhancing sensory processing of the attended target stimulus, the expense of processing unattended distractors. The second mechanism involves enhancing choice bias, i.e., providing relatively greater weightage to the target stimulus in the downstream decision process, and filtering out distractors. Conventional signal detection theory models permit quantifying sensitivity and bias in simple perceptual tasks, but are insufficient to estimate sensitivity and bias in attention tasks. A recently developed psychophysical framework, the m-ADC framework, enables quantifying sensitivity and bias in attention tasks. This framework could be used, in conjunction with TMS, to identify the specific neural bases of sensitivity or bias control during top-down or bottom-up attention tasks.125


    Transcranial electrical stimulation protocols typically produce long lasting effects (minutes to hours) on brain activity; concurrent TMS-tES is increasingly applied for investigating neural mechanisms of these tES effects. In such approaches, tES is used to modulate the underlying activity of a region of interest over long timescales (minutes to hours), and TMS (e.g., single-pulse or paired-pulse protocols) is used to transiently probe and quantify these modulatory effects. Because a single pulse of TMS over motor cortex readily evokes a ready readout of muscle excitability, in the form of the motor-evoked potential (MEP), such approaches have been extensively used to investigate motor cortex function, including investigations of corticocortical connectivity and corticospinal excitability.128 , 129 For instance, Feurra et al. applied 10min of anodal tDCS stimulation of the right parietal cortex during a motor imagery and observation task. They showed that tDCS enhanced MEPs, corresponding to an increase in excitability, of the ipsilateral motor cortex during the motor imagery task.129 More recently, Nowak et al. employed a combined TMS-tACS paradigm to illustrate how gamma frequency tACS over the motor cortex, enhanced intracortical inhibition assessed by paired-pulse TMS stimulation.130 While such combinatorial stimulation paradigms have not yet found extensive use in the study of attention, combined TMS-tACS paradigms may help identify key mechanisms by which specific brain rhythms, as induced by tACS, alter neural excitability, as probed by TMS, during top-down or bottom-up attention tasks.


    Electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings reveal the dynamics of neuroelectric processes occurring fine timescales (few milliseconds); concurrent TMS-EEG protocols provide a sensitive readout of the effects of neurostimulation. Even when behavioral effects are not readily apparent, TMS produces systematic effects on the EEG.37 TMS effects on sensory or motor components of the sự kiện-related potential (ERP) are well studied; these effects vary with the site of stimulation.131,132,133,134, 135 Typically, low-frequency (<1Hz) rhythmic TMS enhances ERP amplitudes and decreases ERP latencies; high-frequency (>5Hz) TMS produces converse effects. Rhythmic stimulation protocols, such as cTBS, have been shown to produce large (~30%) and sustained (1570min) decreases in ERP amplitudes, thereby providing a neural basis for the effects of these protocols on behavior.37 A few concurrent TMS-EEG studies have investigated the roles of the prefrontal and parietal cortex in the context of attention. Captosto et al. explored the effects of applying a single rTMS train of 20Hz for 150ms on the right FEF and IPS of subjects performing a stimulus detection task, in the anticipatory period attention cue onset and before target onset.136 The authors observed that anticipatory alpha rhythms in the occipital lobe were desynchronized and identification of the target was impaired. Similarly, ERP coupled rTMS (5 pulses 10Hz) applied to the right FEF between cue and target onset in a top-down attention task produced a negative deflection both in cue-evoked and target-evoked ERPs.137 Finally, single-pulse TMS of the right PPC during a conjunction task 100ms after array onset produced a delay in reaction times that corresponded to the disruption of a 250-300ms component of a visual ERP.99 EEG-TMS provides a powerful tool for measuring the electrophysiological correlates of stimulating particular brain areas, and may be crucial for temporally precise probing of the mechanistic involvement of fast neural processes (e.g., ERPs or neural oscillations) in top-down and bottom-up attention tasks.


    Finally, combining TMS with fMRI provides an unprecedented opportunity for precise stimulation of functionally defined brain regions, as well as for visualizing the effects of TMS on neural activity across the whole brain.138 In an fMRI-informed TMS study Sack et al.139 acquired fMRI, while subjects performed a target detection task, based on feature conjunctions, followed by TMS (Fig.3ac). Left and right IPS loci that showed activation during task performance were then targeted for rTMS (600 pulses 1Hz). rTMS disrupted performance by increasing reaction times compared to control conditions, linking IPS neural activity to top-down visuospatial detection.139 Concurrent TMS-fMRI permits investigating local effects of TMS the site of stimulation as well as global effects in areas connected by long-range projection fibers to the area being stimulated. Several studies have employed fMRI to measure alterations of brain activity in sensory areas following TMS of attention-related areas. Ruff et al. applied rTMS (5 pulses 10Hz) to the right FEF and observed an increase in the BOLD response of retinotopic visual areas; the stimulation also produced an increase in the apparent (perceived) contrast of peripheral stimuli.140 Blankenburg et al. applied 10Hz rTMS (5 pulses) the PPC, during a top-down discrimination task, and observed that certain occipito-parietal areas that showed higher activation during attention to contralateral than ipsilateral stimuli in control conditions underwent an increase in the magnitude of this differential activation upon stimulation. Behavioral performance, though, remained unaltered.141 TMS-fMRI has also provided putative evidence for dissociable roles of the PFC and PPC in attention to motion features142: TMS over the IPS, but not FEF, produced a reduced BOLD response to motion stimuli in area MT.142


    As demonstrated by these findings, emerging combinatorial paradigms are likely to be invaluable in the search for specific neural mechanisms of top-down versus bottom-up attention. Nevertheless, significant technical challenges must be overcome before these paradigms can be widely adopted, as discussed in the next section.


    Challenges


    Despite the promise of TMS, and combinatorial TMS-imaging paradigms, several key challenges, both with the application of these techniques and the interpretation of findings, remain. We discuss these here along with recent technological advances that have provided a starting point for addressing these challenges.


    First, TMS effects are variable across individuals. The physiological effects of TMS depend heavily on the precise structural topography of the brain region being stimulated, including the depth and angle of gyral folds relative to the orientation of the TMS coil, the density and orientation of axons and cell bodies, the size of and inter-connectivity within the brain region, and the like. Unsurprisingly, significant variability has been reported in the size, duration and even direction of TMSs behavioral effects across individuals, likely due to inter-individual differences in brain structure.31 This limits reproducibility across studies, especially those testing small cohorts of individuals for subtle behavioral effects, as is common in attention tasks. Conventionally, the positioning of TMS coils is guided by sophisticated neuro-navigation algorithms that rely on high resolution structural MRI scans to align the TMS coil particular positions and orientations to target specific brain areas in each subject. Advances in neuro-navigation algorithms permit constructing more sophisticated head models to guide more precise positioning of the TMS coil and to get more accurate estimates of the spread of stimulation across adjacent brain areas.13 Moreover, recent advances in diffusion imaging techniques (dMRI) permit measuring inter-individual differences in brain structural connectivity that might underlie differences in TMS effects. For instance, a recent study showed that differences in connectivity across the corpus callosum were predictive of inter-individual differences in behavioral accuracy, presumably due to differences in interhemispheric coordination.143 In addition, TMS effects are also highly sensitive to stimulation parameters. For instance, oscillatory entrainment in parietal or frontal areas does not occur reliably even with minor differences in the stimulation protocol in terms of stimulation site, alignment of the frequency and phase of the applied TMS with respect to inherent oscillations and the like, again potentially due also to inter-individual differences in physiology.115 , 128 To partly address this source of variability, recent studies have applied rhythmic neurostimulation by matching stimulation frequencies for each subject to her/his frequency of natural brain oscillations, as recorded by EEG/MEG.115


    Second, it is increasingly clear that the effects of TMS also vary depending on the timing of stimulation relative to the underlying neural state. Converging evidence indicates that stimulation immediately prior to the onset of stimulus facilitates behavioral performance, while stimulation during stimulus presentation usually disrupts performance. For instance, stimulation preceding stimulus onset improved object naming latency and target detection efficiency 93 , 144, whereas stimulation after target onset in motion discrimination and target discrimination tasks reduced discrimination accuracy.38 , 103 , 145 These results have been explained as follows: if TMS were to enhance the excitability of neural populations in the quiescent state, TMS delivered just before the stimulus may enhance neural excitability, and improve stimulus processing thereby facilitating behavioral performance. On the other hand, during an ongoing task, when task-relevant neural populations are already highly active ( ceiling), TMS may increase the excitability of task-inhibitory or task-irrelevant neural populations thereby disrupting processing and behavioral performance. These state-dependent effects manifest in other forms as well. For instance, Silvanto et al. showed that TMS on FEF produced phosphenes with the color of the stimuli of a previous color adaptation task, when the stimulation followed the adaptation task.146 This state-dependent effect of TMS can be utilized for selective analysis of neural populations. By preventing the activation of neurons that encode specific stimulus features, for example, with sensory adaptation, TMSs effects on the non-adapted populations of neurons can be selectively investigated.146 These approaches could provide essential insights into the role of specific neural populations that encode particular features (e.g., orientation or color) in mediating the effects of top-down and bottom-up attention.


    Third, although TMS has been widely used for stimulating cortical tissue (23cm) from the scalp surface) with conventional coils, it is not ideal for stimulating deeper, sub-cortical structures. Even in cases where higher stimulation strengths have been used to stimulate deeper structures (e.g., 120% of motor threshold147), collateral activation of neural tissue superficial to these structures is inevitable. Nevertheless, gaining a full understanding of cognitive processes, like attention, demands the ability to stimulate deeper brain structures like the superior colliculus and basal ganglia. To achieve stimulation of deeper brain structures, recent studies have employed an indirect approachby stimulating cortical connections that share strong anatomical connectivity with these sub-cortical areas, and confirming their indirect activation with concurrent fMRI. For example, Wang et al. applied high-frequency rTMS to the lateral parietal cortex, part of a cortical-hippocampal network and demonstrated enhanced functional connectivity within this network as well as improved associative memory performance.148 An important point to note with this approach is that it is difficult to dissociate the effects of indirect modulation of the sub-cortical structure from those of the direct modulation of the cortical region, and the results must be interpreted as arising from activation of a distributed network. Recent improvements in hardware seek to achieve focal stimulation of deeper brain areas through advanced coil designs (e.g., H1/H2 coils).149 Although these are being developed primarily for therapeutic purposes, once commercialized, these have the potential to provide important insights into the role of key sub-cortical brain structures in top-down and bottom-up attention.


    Fourth, although emerging combinatorial paradigms permit more precise evaluation of the neural mechanisms of TMS; these also come with important technical challenges. For example, it remains a significant challenge to analyze EEG signals recorded concurrently with TMS, because of the large TMS-induced artifacts in the EEG signal (but see150). Similarly, concurrent TMS-fMRI involves taking into consideration important safety issues associated with performing TMS inside MR scanner: the strong magnetic field and switching gradients within the MRI scanner bore can cause heating up and unsafe temperature rise in the TMS coil. In addition, the large TMS magnetic field gradients can induce significant artifacts in fMRI recordings.151 , 152 These challenges need to be overcome before combinatorial paradigms can find widespread application.


    Fifth, despite decades of study, the precise neurophysiological mechanisms of TMS remain unclear. Animal model studies are constrained due to the necessity of scaling the coil size correspondingly to the head size to maintain stimulation efficiency153, although efforts are being made to overcome some of these challenges.154 Additionally, advances in computational modeling of stimulation effects and integration of TMS with functional imaging and spectroscopy may provide greater insights into the precise mechanisms of stimulation.155


    Finally, TMS faces a key challenge, one also shared by other brain stimulation techniques, in terms of interpreting the effects of stimulation. The conventional approach to neuroscience emphasizes understanding the functional role of particular neural populations in specific brain regions. Hence, neurostimulation techniques have traditionally sought to study the effect of spatially focal stimulation of particular brain regions (e.g., with TMS) or of specific neural groups (e.g., with optogenetics). However, this view is changing rapidly: it is increasingly clear that cognitive processes, like attention, require the coordinated activity of neural populations across multiple brain regions. Thus, behavioral effects of TMS, or indeed, of any neurostimulation technique, are likely to arise not from altered activity of an isolated neural population, but rather as a consequence of concurrent changes in several connected neural populations across brain regions acting together as functionally coupled networks. Taking these network effects into consideration is crucial for fully interpreting the results of stimulation studies, particularly those that seek to understand the neural underpinnings of complex cognitive phenomena. Recent work in network neuroscience, based on network control theory, has begun to address key challenges produced by this emerging perspective of brain function.156


    Conclusions and Future Directions


    While invasive neurostimulation studies in non-human primates, and other mammals, have provided key insights into attention mechanisms, recent advancements in non-invasive technologies, and combinatorial paradigms, have opened up new frontiers for testing these mechanisms in the human brain. Humans can be readily trained to perform complex attentional tasks, and human TMS studies carry the advantage of being able to evaluate subtle differences between top-down and bottom-up attention control using sophisticated task designs. Moreover, mechanisms of attention control, as discovered with human TMS studies, can form the basis for constructing further, detailed hypotheses of how these mechanisms operate the level of neural circuits. These models may then be tested with cellular and circuit-level manipulations (e.g., optogenetics) in non-human primate and other animal models.


    Of particular interest is the role of fronto-parietal areas in mediating top-down and bottom-up attention. There is active research and debate on the distinct involvement of these areas and their timing of activation during the two modes of attention.106 , 109 , 137 Receptive field sizes of the frontal and parietal regions are much larger (over 100°) as compared to early sensory coding areas (e.g., V10.5° to 2°). The regions also have extensive reciprocal connections with many upstream executive control regions, such as DLPFC and downstream sensory regions, such as MT, V4 and V1.41 , 85 This allows these regions to accumulate information across the visual field to form a bottom-up saliency map, and then integrate this with top-down goals to form a priority map of the environment.157 , 158 How the saliency and priority maps are combined to determine the next target for the allocation of visual attention, either top-down or bottom-up, remains an active area of research,72 and TMS can shed light on the distinct involvement of the frontal and parietal regions in computing and integrating these maps.


    In this review, we have focused primarily on mechanisms of top-down and bottom-up visual spatial attention. However, these two modes of attention control certainly operate for other forms of attention (e.g., attention to features or objects) and other sensory modalities (e.g., audition). MEG recordings in humans have shown that the inferior frontal gyrus is critically important, and could mediate selection of relevant stimuli during object-based attention.159 Non-invasive neurostimulation will be key to unraveling whether brain regions and neural mechanisms for object-based or auditory attention control are shared with those for top-down and bottom-up visuospatial attention.160


    TMS is also particularly important in terms of its translational potential. TMS has found wide use in therapeutic interventions for treating neuropsychological disorders like drug resistant epilepsy, Parkinsons disease and major depressive disorder. Low-frequency rTMS applied for 1530min can be used to suppress seizure generating excitatory activity in epileptic foci as well as abate extant seizure activity. rTMS has also been used for alleviating gamma oscillation deficits in autism and schizophrenia.161 , 162 rTMS sessions in rats were shown to induce plastic molecular changesincluding changes in the levels c-fos, glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), cholecystokinin or corticotropin (ACTH)resembling the effects of antidepressants or electroconvulsive therapy interventions.26 , 163 Novel TMS techniques to focally stimulate particular deep brain regions are being developed for medical use and clinical trials. Together with emerging imaging technologies, TMS can pave the way toward a more complete understanding of the neural basis of selective attention, as well as other cognitive phenomena, both in health and in disease.


    References


  • 1.

    Deuschl G (2006) A randomized trial of deep-brain stimulation for Parkinson. N Engl J Med 355:896908


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 2.

    Lulic D, Ahmadian A, Baaj AA, Benbadis SR, Vale FL (2009) Vagus nerve stimulation. Neurosurg Focus 27:E5


    Article Google Scholar



  • 3.

    Loeser JD, Black RG, Christman A (1975) Relief of pain by transcutaneous stimulation. J Neurosurg 42:308314


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 4.

    Antal A, Paulus W (2013) Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Front Hum Neurosci 7:14


    Article Google Scholar



  • 5.

    Stagg CJ et al (2011) Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia 49:800804


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 6.

    Heinen K et al (2022) Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over posterior parietal cortex enhances distinct aspects of visual working memory. Neuropsychologia 87:3542


    Article Google Scholar



  • 7.

    Helfrich RF et al (2015) Selective modulation of interhemispheric functional connectivity by HD-tACS shapes perception. PLoS Biol 12:115


    Google Scholar



  • 8.

    Vossen A, Gross J, Thut G (2015) Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation alpha frequency (α-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul 8:499508


    Article Google Scholar



  • 9.

    Sparing R, Mottaghy FM (2008) Noninvasive brain stimulation with transcranial magnetic or direct current stimulation (TMS/tDCS)from insights into human memory to therapy of its dysfunction. Methods 44:329337


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 10.

    Barker AT, Jalinous R, Freeston IL (1985) Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Lancet 325(8437):11061107


    Article Google Scholar



  • 11.

    Rossini PM, Rossi S (2007) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: diagnostic, therapeutic, and research potential. Neurology 68:484488


    Article Google Scholar



  • 12.

    Chambers CD, Heinen K (2010) TMS and the functional neuroanatomy of attention. Cortex 46:114117


    Article Google Scholar



  • 13.

    Deng ZD, Lisanby SH, Peterchev AV (2013) Electric field depthfocality tradeoff in transcranial magnetic stimulation: simulation comparison of 50 coil designs. Brain Stimul 6(1):113


    Article Google Scholar



  • 14.

    Amassian VE et al (1989) Suppression of visual perception by magnetic coil stimulation of human occipital cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Potent Sect 74:458462


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 15.

    Amassian VE, Cracco RQ, Maccabee PJ (1989) Focal stimulation of human cerebral cortex with the magnetic coil: a comparison with electrical stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Potent Sect 74:401416


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 16.

    Day BL, Dressler D, Maertens de Noordhout A, Marsden CD, Nakashima K, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD (1989) Electric and magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex: surface EMG and single motor unit responses. J Physiol 412(1):449473


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 17.

    Pascual-Leone A, Cohen LG, Brasil-Neto JP, Hallett M (1994) Non-invasive differentiation of motor cortical representation of hand muscles by mapping of optimal current directions. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Potent Sect 93:4248


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 18.

    Wassermann EM et al (1996) Use and safety of a new repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulator. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Mot Control 101:412417


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 19.

    Maeda F, Keenan JP, Tormos JM, Topka H, Pascual-Leone A (2000) Interindividual variability of the modulatory effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical excitability. Exp Brain Res 133:425430


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 20.

    Pascual-Leone A, Tormos JM, Keenan J, Tarazona F, Cañete C, Catalá MD (1998) Study and modulation of human cortical excitability with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Clin Neurophys 15(4):333343


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 21.

    Cole JC, Green Bernacki C, Helmer A, Pinninti N, Oreardon JP (2015) Efficacy of Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in the treatment of schizophrenia: a review of the literature to date. Innov Clin Neurosci 12:1219


    Google Scholar



  • 22.

    Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V, Rothwell J (2000) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neurosciencevirtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10:232237


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 23.

    Grafman J, Pascual-Leone A, Alway D, Nichelli P, Gomez-Tortosa E, Hallett M (1994) Induction of a recall deficit by rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroreport 5(9):11571160



  • 24.

    Chambers CD, Mattingley JB (2005) Neurodisruption of selective attention: insights and implications. Trends Cogn Sci 9:542550


    Article Google Scholar



  • 25.

    Rushworth MFS et al (2002) Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. J Neurophysiol 87:25772592


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 26.

    Ogiue-Ikeda M, Kawato S, Ueno S (2003) The effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on long-term potentiation in rat hippocampus depends on stimulus intensity. Brain Res 993:222226


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 27.

    Thielscher A, Opitz A, Windhoff M (2011) Impact of the gyral geometry on the electric field induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 54(1): 234243



  • 28.

    Windhoff M, Opitz A, Thielscher A (2013) Electric field calculations in brain stimulation based on finite elements: an optimized processing pipeline for the generation and usage of accurate individual head models. Hum Brain Mapp 34:923935


    Article Google Scholar



  • 29.

    Opitz A, Windhoff M, Heidemann RM, Turner R, Thielscher A (2011) How the brain tissue shapes the electric field induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 58:849859


    Article Google Scholar



  • 30.

    Ziemann U et al (1998) Demonstration of facilitatory I wave interaction in the human motor cortex by paired transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Physiol 511:181190


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 31.

    Wagner T, Rushmore J, Eden U, Valero-Cabre A (2009) Biophysical foundations underlying TMS: setting the stage for an effective use of neurostimulation in the cognitive neurosciences. Cortex 45(9):10251034


    Article Google Scholar



  • 32.

    Huang Y-Z, Chen R-S, Rothwell JC, Wen H-Y (2007) The after-effect of human theta burst stimulation is NMDA receptor dependent. Clin Neurophysiol 118:10281032


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 33.

    Muller PA et al (2014) Suppression of motor cortical excitability in anesthetized rats by low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS One 9:18


    Google Scholar



  • 34.

    Huang Y-Z, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC (2005) Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45:201206


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 35.

    Smith DT, Jackson SR, Rorden C (2005) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left human frontal eye fields eliminates the cost of invalid endogenous cues. Neuropsychologia 43:12881296


    Article Google Scholar



  • 36.

    Meister IG et al (2006) Hemiextinction induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuroscience 142:119123


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 37.

    Thut G, Pascual-Leone A (2010) Editorial: integrating TMS with EEG: how and what for? Brain Topogr 22:215218


    Article Google Scholar



  • 38.

    Silvanto J, Muggleton NG (2008) New light through old windows: moving beyond the virtual lesion approach to transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 39:549552


    Article Google Scholar



  • 39.

    Dugué L, Marque P, VanRullen R (2015) Theta oscillations modulate attentional search performance periodically. J Cogn Neurosci



  • 40.

    Sakai K, Ugawa Y, Terao Y, Hanajima R, Furubayashi T, Kanazawa I (1997) Preferential activation of different I waves by transcranial magnetic stimulation with a figure-of-eight-shaped coil. Exp Brain Res 113(1):2432



  • 41.

    Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:215229


    Article CAS Google Scholar



  • 42.

    Ling S, Carrasco M (2006) Sustained and transient covert attention enhance the signal via different contrast response functions. Vis Res 46:12101220


    Article Google Scholar



  • 43.

    Carrasco M (2011) Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vis Res 51:14841525


    Article Google Scholar



  • 44.

    Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q. J Exp Psychol 32:325


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 45.

    Müller HJ, Findlay JM (1987) Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention. Percept Psychophys 42:383399


    Article Google Scholar



  • 46.

    Liu T, Pestilli F, Carrasco M (2005) Transient attention enhances perceptual performance and fMRI response in human visual cortex. Neuron 45:469477


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 47.

    Liu T, Fuller S, Carrasco M (2006) Attention alters the appearance of motion coherence. Psychon Bull Rev 13:10911096


    Article Google Scholar



  • 48.

    Wagner T, Valero-Cabre A, Pascual-Leone A (2007) Noninvasive human brain stimulation. Annu Rev Biomed Eng 9:527565


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 49.

    Carrasco M, Yeshurun Y (1998) The contribution of covert attention to the set-size and eccentricity effects in visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:673692


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 50.

    Yeshurun Y, Carrasco M (2000) The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation. Nat Neurosci 3:622627


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 51.

    Herrmann K, Montaser-Kouhsari L, Carrasco M, Heeger DJ (2010) When size matters: attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nat Neurosci 13:15541559


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 52.

    Chica AB, Martín-Arévalo E, Botta F, Lupiáñez J (2014) The spatial orienting paradigm: how to design and interpret spatial attention experiments. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 40:3551


    Article Google Scholar



  • 53.

    Pestilli F, Carrasco M (2005) Attention enhances contrast sensitivity cued and impairs it uncued locations. Vis Res 45:18671875


    Article Google Scholar



  • 54.

    Luck SJ, Hillyard SA, Mouloua M, Hawkins HL (1996) Mechanisms of visualspatial attention: resource allocation or uncertainty reduction? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:725737


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 55.

    Beck DM, Kastner S (2009) Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vis Res 49:11541165


    Article Google Scholar



  • 56.

    Desimone R, Duncan JS (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18:193222


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 57.

    Foley JM, Schwarz W (1998) Spatial attention: effect of position uncertainty and number of distractor patterns on the threshold-versus-contrast function for contrast discrimination. J Opt Soc Am A 15:10361047


    Article Google Scholar



  • 58.

    Yeshurun Y, Montagna B, Carrasco M (2008) On the flexibility of sustained attention and its effects on a texture segmentation task. Vis Res 48:8095


    Article Google Scholar



  • 59.

    Carrasco M, McElree B, Denisova K, Giordano AM (2003) Speed of visual processing increases with eccentricity. Nat Neurosci 6:699700


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 60.

    Ivanoff J, Klein RM (2004) Stimulus-response probability and inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 11:542550


    Article Google Scholar



  • 61.

    Chica AB, Bartolomeo P, Lupiáñez J (2013) Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention. Behav Brain Res 237:107123


    Article Google Scholar



  • 62.

    Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ (2009) The normalization model of attention. Neuron 61:168185


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 63.

    Yeshurun Y, Levy L (2003) Transient spatial attention degrades temporal resolution. Psychol Sci 14:225231


    Article Google Scholar



  • 64.

    Wolfe JM, Butcher SJ, Lee C, Hyle M (2003) Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:483502


    Article Google Scholar



  • 65.

    Connor CE, Egeth HE, Yantis S (2004) Visual attention: bottom-up versus top-down. Curr Biol 14:850852


    Article CAS Google Scholar



  • 66.

    McPeek RM, Keller EL (2004) Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus. Nat Neurosci 7:757763


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 67.

    Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL, Petersen SE (1993) A PET study of visuospatial attention. J Neurosci 13:12021226


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 68.

    Fielding J, Georgiou-Karistianis N, White O (2006) The role of the basal ganglia in the control of automatic visuospatial attention. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 12:657667


    Google Scholar



  • 69.

    Mesulam M-M (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10:309325


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 70.

    Cohen MR, Maunsell JHR (2009) Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations. Nat Neurosci 12:15941600


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 71.

    Kastner S, Ungerleider LG (2000) Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:315341


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 72.

    Knudsen EI (2007) Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:5778


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 73.

    Peelen MV, Heslenfeld DJ, Theeuwes J (2004) Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network. Neuroimage 22:822830


    Article Google Scholar



  • 74.

    Hahn B, Ross TJ, Stein EA (2006) Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom-up and top-down processes of visuospatial selective attention. Neuroimage 32:842853


    Article Google Scholar



  • 75.

    Kincade JM, Abrams RA, Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2005) An sự kiện-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. J Neurosci 25:45934604


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 76.

    Buschman TJ, Miller EK (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315(5820):18601862


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 77.

    Busse L, Katzner S, Treue S (2008) Temporal dynamics of neuronal modulation during exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention in macaque area MT. Proc Natl Acad Sci 105:1638016385


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 78.

    Katsuki F, Constantinidis C (2014) Bottom-up and top-down attention. Neuroscience 20:509521


    Article Google Scholar



  • 79.

    Moore T, Fallah M (2001) Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:12731276


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 80.

    Moore T, Armstrong KM (2003) Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature 421:370373


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 81.

    Ibos G, Duhamel JR, Hamed SB (2013) A functional hierarchy within the parietofrontal network in stimulus selection and attention control. J Neurosci 33(19):83598369



  • 82.

    Katsuki F (2012) Unique and shared roles of the posterior parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitive functions. Front Integr Neurosci 6:113


    Article Google Scholar



  • 83.

    Grent-t-Jong T, Woldorff MG (2007) Timing and sequence of brain activity in top-down control of visual-spatial attention. PLoS Biol 5:01140126


    Article CAS Google Scholar



  • 84.

    Katsuki F, Constantinidis C (2012) Early involvement of prefrontal cortex in visual bottom-up attention. Nat Neurosci 15:11601166


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 85.

    Schall JD, Paré M, Woodman GF (2007) Comment onTop-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science (80-.) 318:44


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 86.

    Ikkai A, Dandekar S, Curtis CE (2022) Lateralization in Alpha-band oscillations predicts the locus and spatial distribution of attention. PLoS One 11:117


    Article CAS Google Scholar



  • 87.

    Gould IC, Rushworth MF, Nobre AC (2011) Indexing the graded allocation of visuospatial attention using anticipatory alpha oscillations. J Neurophysiol 105:13181326


    Article Google Scholar



  • 88.

    Thut G, Nietzel A, Brandt SA, Pascual-Leone A (2006) α-band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. J Neurosci 26:94949502


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 89.

    Worden MS, Foxe JJ, Wang N, Simpson GV (2000) Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J Neurosci 20:RC63


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 90.

    Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Hénaff M-A, Isnard J, Fischer C (2005) Attention modulates gamma-band oscillations differently in the human lateral occipital cortex and fusiform gyrus. Cereb Cortex 15:654662


    Article Google Scholar



  • 91.

    Wyart V, Tallon-Baudry C (2008) Neural dissociation between visual awareness and spatial attention. J Neurosci 28:26672679


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 92.

    Landau AN, Esterman M, Robertson LC, Bentin S, Prinzmetal W (2007) Different effects of voluntary and involuntary attention on EEG activity in the gamma band. J Neurosci 27:1198611990


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 93.

    Grosbras M-H, Paus T (2003) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field facilitates visual awareness. Eur J Neurosci 18:31213126


    Article Google Scholar



  • 94.

    Grosbras M-H, Paus T (2002) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field: effects on visual perception and attention. J Cogn Neurosci 14:11091120


    Article Google Scholar



  • 95.

    Neggers SFW et al (2007) TMS pulses on the frontal eye fields break coupling between visuospatial attention and eye movements. J Neurophysiol 98:27652778


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 96.

    Turatto M, Sandrini M, Miniussi C (2004) The role of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in visual change awareness. Neuroreport 15:25492552


    Article Google Scholar



  • 97.

    Kalla R, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V (2009) Human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in visual search for conjunctions but not features: a theta TMS study. Cortex 45:10851090


    Article Google Scholar



  • 98.

    Muggleton NG, Juan C-H, Cowey A, Walsh V, OBreathnach U (2010) Human frontal eye fields and target switching. Cortex 46:178184


    Article Google Scholar



  • 99.

    Fuggetta G (2006) Cortico-cortical interactions in spatial attention: a combined ERP/TMS study. J Neurophysiol 95:32773280


    Article Google Scholar



  • 100.

    Thut G, Nietzel A, Pascual-Leone A (2005) Dorsal posterior parietal rTMS affects voluntary orienting of visuospatial attention. Cereb Cortex 15:628638


    Article Google Scholar



  • 101.

    Beck DM, Muggleton N, Walsh V, Lavie N (2006) Right parietal cortex plays a critical role in change blindness. Cereb Cortex 16:712717


    Article Google Scholar



  • 102.

    Ashbridge E, Walsh V, Cowey A (1997) Temporal aspects of visual search studied by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia 35:11211131


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 103.

    OShea J, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V (2004) Timing of target discrimination in human frontal eye fields. J Cogn Neurosci 16:10601067


    Article Google Scholar



  • 104.

    Kalla R, Muggleton NG, Juan C-H, Cowey A, Walsh V (2008) The timing of the involvement of the frontal eye fields and posterior parietal cortex in visual search. NeuroReport 19:10671071


    Article Google Scholar



  • 105.

    Müri R et al (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry in visuospatial attention assessed with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp Brain Res 143:426430


    Article Google Scholar



  • 106.

    Chambers CD, Payne JM, Stokes MG, Mattingley JB (2004) Fast and slow parietal pathways mediate spatial attention. Nat Neurosci 7:217218


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 107.

    Dambeck N et al (2006) Interhemispheric imbalance during visuospatial attention investigated by unilateral and bilateral TMS over human parietal cortices. Brain Res 1072:194199


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 108.

    Hilgetag CC, Théoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2001) Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced virtual lesions of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:953957


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 109.

    Heinen K et al (2011) Concurrent TMS-fMRI reveals dynamic interhemispheric influences of the right parietal cortex during exogenously cued visuospatial attention. Eur J Neurosci 33:9911000


    Article Google Scholar



  • 110.

    Capotosto P, Babiloni C, Romani GL, Corbetta M (2012) Differential contribution of right and left parietal cortex to the control of spatial attention: a simultaneous EEGrTMS study. Cereb Cortex 22:446454


    Article Google Scholar



  • 111.

    Krall SC et al (2022) The right temporoparietal junction in attention and social interaction: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Hum Brain Mapp 37:796807


    Article Google Scholar



  • 112.

    Bestmann S, Ruff CC, Blakemore C, Driver J, Thilo KV (2007) Spatial attention changes excitability of human visual cortex to direct stimulation. Curr Biol 17:134139


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 113.

    Silvanto J, Muggleton N, Lavie N, Walsh V (2009) The perceptual and functional consequences of parietal top-down modulation on the visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 19:327330


    Article Google Scholar



  • 114.

    Sauseng P, Feldheim JF, Freunberger R, Hummel FC (2011) Right prefrontal TMS disrupts interregional anticipatory EEG alpha activity during shifting of visuospatial attention. Front Psychol 2:19


    Article Google Scholar



  • 115.

    Thut G, Veniero D, Romei V, Miniussi C, Schyns P, Gross J (2011) Rhythmic TMS causes local entrainment of natural oscillatory signatures. Curr Biol 21(14):11761185


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 116.

    Dugué L, Roberts M, Carrasco M (2022) Attention reorients periodically. Curr Biol 26(12):15951601



  • 117.

    Marshall TR, O’Shea J, Jensen O, Bergmann TO (2015) Frontal eye fields control attentional modulation of alpha and gamma oscillations in contralateral occipitoparietal cortex. J Neurosci 35(4):16381647



  • 118.

    Herring JD, Thut G, Jensen O, Bergmann TO (2015) Attention modulates TMS-locked alpha oscillations in the visual cortex. J Neurosci 35:1443514447


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 119.

    Romei V, Driver J, Schyns PG, Thut G (2011) Rhythmic TMS over parietal cortex links distinct brain frequencies to global versus local visual processing. Curr Biol 21:334337


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 120.

    Chanes L, Quentin R, Tallon-Baudry C, Valero-Cabré A, Valero-Cabre A (2013) Causal frequency-specific contributions of frontal spatiotemporal patterns induced by non-invasive neurostimulation to human visual performance. J Neurosci 33:50005005


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 121.

    Romei V, Thut G, Silvanto J (2022) Information-based approaches of noninvasive transcranial brain stimulation. Trends Neurosci 39:782795


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 122.

    Wagenmakers E-J, Grasman RPPP, Molenaar PCM (2005) On the relation between the mean and the variance of a diffusion model response time distribution. J Math Psychol 49:195204


    Article Google Scholar



  • 123.

    Donkin C, Brown S, Heathcote A, Wagenmakers E-J (2011) Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: different models but the same conclusions about psychological processes? Psychon Bull Rev 18:6169


    Article Google Scholar



  • 124.

    Forstmann BU, Ratcliff R, Wagenmakers E-J (2022) Sequential sampling models in cognitive neuroscience: advantages, applications, and extensions. Annu Rev Psychol 67:641666


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 125.

    Sridharan D, Steinmetz NNA, Moore T, Knudsen EI (2014) Distinguishing bias from sensitivity effects in multialternative detection tasks. J Vis 14:16


    Article Google Scholar



  • 126.

    Eckstein MP, Thomas JP, Palmer J, Shimozaki SS (2000) A signal detection model predicts the effects of set size on visual search accuracy for feature, conjunction, triple conjunction, and disjunction displays. Percept Psychophys 62:425451


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 127.

    Sridharan D, Steinmetz NA, Moore T, Knudsen EI (2022) Does the superior colliculus control perceptual sensitivity or choice bias during attention? Evidence from a multialternative decision framework. J Neurosci 37:480511


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 128.

    Bergmann TO et al (2009) Acute changes in motor cortical excitability during slow oscillatory and constant anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. J Neurophysiol 102:23032311


    Article Google Scholar



  • 129.

    Feurra M, Paulus W, Walsh V, Kanai R (2011) Frequency specific modulation of human somatosensory cortex. Front Psychol 2:13


    Article Google Scholar



  • 130.

    Nowak M, Hinson E, van Ede F, Pogosyan A, Guerra A, Quinn A, Brown P, Stagg CJ (2022) Driving human motor cortical oscillations leads to behaviorally relevant changes in local GABAA inhibition: a tACS-TMS study. J Neurosci 37(17):44814492


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 131.

    Katayama T, Rothwell JC (2007) Modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials using transcranial magnetic intermittent theta burst stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol 118:25062511


    Article Google Scholar



  • 132.

    Ferreri F, Ponzo D, Hukkanen T, Mervaala E, Könönen M, Pasqualetti P, Vecchio F, Rossini PM, Määttä S (2012) Human brain cortical correlates of short-latency afferent inhibition: a combined EEGTMS study. J Neurophysiol 108(1):314323


    Article Google Scholar



  • 133.

    Restuccia D, Ulivelli M, De Capua A, Bartalini S, Rossi S (2007) Modulation of high-frequency (600Hz) somatosensory-evoked potentials after rTMS of the primary sensory cortex. Eur J Neurosci 26:23492358


    Article Google Scholar



  • 134.

    Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A (2012) Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin Neurophysiol 120:323330


    Google Scholar



  • 135.

    Ortu E, Ruge D, Deriu F, Rothwell JC (2009) Theta burst stimulation over the human primary motor cortex modulates neural processes involved in movement preparation. Clin Neurophysiol 120:11951203


    Article Google Scholar



  • 136.

    Capotosto P, Babiloni C, Romani GL, Corbetta M (2009) Frontoparietal cortex controls spatial attention through modulation of anticipatory alpha rhythms. J Neurosci 29(18):58635872



  • 137.

    Taylor PCJ, Nobre AC, Rushworth MFS (2007) FEF TMS affects visual cortical activity. Cereb Cortex 17:391399


    Article Google Scholar



  • 138.

    Bohning DE et al (1998) Echoplanar BOLD fMRI of brain activation induced by concurrent transcranial magnetic stimulation. Invest Radiol 33



  • 139.

    Sack AT et al (2002) The experimental combination of rTMS and fMRI reveals the functional relevance of parietal cortex for visuospatial functions. Cogn Brain Res 13:8593


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 140.

    Ruff CC et al (2006) Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on human retinotopic visual cortex. Curr Biol 16:14791488


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 141.

    Blankenburg F et al (2010) Studying the role of human parietal cortex in visuospatial attention with concurrent TMS-fMRI. Cereb Cortex 20:27022711


    Article Google Scholar



  • 142.

    Ruff CC et al (2008) Distinct causal influences of parietal versus frontal areas on human visual cortex: evidence from concurrent TMSfMRI. Cereb Cortex 18:817827


    Article Google Scholar



  • 143.

    Westerhausen R, Grüner R, Specht K, Hugdahl K (2009) Functional relevance of interindividual differences in temporal lobe callosal pathways: a DTI tractography study. Cereb Cortex 19:13221329


    Article Google Scholar



  • 144.

    Töpper R, Mottaghy FM, Brügmann M, Noth J, Huber W (1998) Facilitation of picture naming by focal transcranial magnetic stimulation of Wernickes area. Exp Brain Res 121:371378


    Article Google Scholar



  • 145.

    Hotson J, Braun D, Herzberg W, Boman D (1994) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of extrastriate cortex degrades human motion direction discrimination. Vis Res 34:21152123


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 146.

    Silvanto J, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V (2007) Neural adaptation reveals state-dependent effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur J Neurosci 25:18741881


    Article Google Scholar



  • 147.

    OReardon JP et al (2007) Efficacy and safety of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the acute treatment of major depression: a multisite randomized controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 62:12081216


    Article Google Scholar



  • 148.

    Wang JX, Rogers LM, Gross EZ, Ryals AJ, Dokucu ME, Brandstatt KL, Hermiller MS, Voss JL (2014) Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks and associative memory. Science 345(6200):10541057



  • 149.

    Harel EV et al (2011) H-coil repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of bipolar depression: an add-on, safety and feasibility study. World J Biol Psychiatry 12:119126


    Article Google Scholar



  • 150.

    Urigüen JA, Garcia-Zapirain B (2015) EEG artifact removalstate-of-the-art and guidelines. J Neural Eng 12:31001


    Article Google Scholar



  • 151.

    Ruff CC, Driver J, Bestmann S (2009) Combining TMS and fMRI: from virtual lesions to functional-network accounts of cognition. Cortex 45:10431049


    Article Google Scholar



  • 152.

    Driver J, Blankenburg F, Bestmann S, Ruff CC (2010) New approaches to the study of human brain networks underlying spatial attention and related processes. Exp Brain Res 206:153162


    Article Google Scholar



  • 153.

    Weissman JD, Epstein CM, Davey KR (1992) Magnetic brain stimulation and brain size: relevance to animal studies. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Potent Sect 85:215219


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 154.

    Tischler H et al (2011) Mini-coil for magnetic stimulation in the behaving primate. J Neurosci Methods 194:242251


    Article Google Scholar



  • 155.

    Herrmann CS, Strüber D, Helfrich RF, Engel AK (2022) EEG oscillations: from correlation to causality. Int J Psychophysiol 103:1221


    Article Google Scholar



  • 156.

    Bassett DS, Bullmore E (2006) Small-world brain networks. Neuroscience 12:512523


    Article Google Scholar



  • 157.

    Bisley JW, Goldberg ME (2010) Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Ann Rev Neurosci 33:121


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 158.

    Fecteau JH, Bell AH, Munoz DP (2004) Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 92(3):17281737



  • 159.

    Baldauf D, Desimone R (2014) Neural mechanisms of object-based attention. Science 344:424427


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 160.

    Schenkluhn B, Ruff CC, Heinen K, Chambers CD (2008) Parietal stimulation decouples spatial and feature-based attention. J Neurosci 28:1110611110


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • 161.

    Farzan F, Barr MS, Sun Y, Fitzgerald PB, Daskalakis ZJ (2012) Transcranial magnetic stimulation on the modulation of gamma oscillations in schizophrenia. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1265:2535


    Article Google Scholar



  • 162.

    Sokhadze EM et al (2009) Effects of low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on gamma frequency oscillations and sự kiện-related potentials during processing of illusory figures in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 39:619634


    Article Google Scholar



  • 163.

    Levkovitz Y, Grisaru N, Segal M (2001) Transcranial magnetic stimulation and antidepressive drugs share similar cellular effects in rat hippocampus. Neuropsychopharmacology 24:608616


    CAS Article Google Scholar



  • Download references


    Acknowledgements


    SB would like to thank Sri Vamsi Matta and Arshed Nabeel for help with making figures. This research was supported by a Wellcome-Trust DBT India Alliance Intermediate Fellowship, a SERB Early Career Award, a Tata Trusts grant and a DBT-IISc partnership program grant (DS).


    Author information


    Affiliations



  • Centre for Neuroscience, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore, 560012, India


    Sanjna Banerjee,Shrey Grover&Devarajan Sridharan



  • Authors


  • Sanjna BanerjeeView author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar



  • Shrey GroverView author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar



  • Devarajan SridharanView author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar



  • Corresponding author


    Correspondence to Devarajan Sridharan.


    Rights and permissions


    Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.


    Reprints and Permissions


    About this article


    Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark


    Cite this article


    Banerjee, S., Grover, S. & Sridharan, D. Unraveling Causal Mechanisms of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Visuospatial Attention with Non-invasive Brain Stimulation. J Indian Inst Sci 97, 451475 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41745-017-0046-0


    Download citation



    • Received: 22 September 2022




    • Accepted: 29 September 2022




    • Published: 06 December 2022




    • Issue Date: December 2022




    • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41745-017-0046-0



    Share this article


    Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:


    Get shareable link


    Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.


    Copy to clipboard


    Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative


    Keywords


    • Combination Paradigm

    • Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS)

    • Neurostimulation

    • Conjunction Search Task

    • Alpha Oscillations

    Reply

    9

    0

    Chia sẻ


    Chia Sẻ Link Download Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices miễn phí


    Bạn vừa đọc nội dung bài viết Với Một số hướng dẫn một cách rõ ràng hơn về Video Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices tiên tiến và phát triển nhất Share Link Down Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices miễn phí.



    Hỏi đáp vướng mắc về Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices


    Nếu sau khi đọc nội dung bài viết Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices vẫn chưa hiểu thì hoàn toàn có thể lại phản hồi ở cuối bài để Mình lý giải và hướng dẫn lại nha

    #Topdown #bottomup #control #attention #prefrontal #posterior #parietal #cortices

*

Đăng nhận xét (0)
Mới hơn Cũ hơn

Responsive Ad

/*! Ads Here */

Billboard Ad

/*! Ads Here */